Skip to main content

Open Access 10.05.2024

Who gets duped? The impact of education on fraud detection in an investment task

verfasst von: Calvin Blackwell, Norman Maynard, James Malm, Mark Pyles, Marcia Snyder, Mark Witte

Erschienen in: Journal of Economics and Finance

Aktivieren Sie unsere intelligente Suche, um passende Fachinhalte oder Patente zu finden.

search-config
loading …

Abstract

Many financial scandals appear to depend on a lack of skepticism on the part of their victims. Sophisticated investors trusted Bernie Madoff, for example, despite early warning signs of implausible returns. Our study investigates how education explains fraud detection in financial decision-making. In a simple survey, economics and finance students are asked to make an investment recommendation from among four hypothetical funds, including one based on Madoff’s fund. We use Truth Default Theory to explain our results. We show that education increases the likelihood that students are suspicious of Madoff’s fund, and that for students whose suspicions are aroused, education makes them less likely to choose the Madoff fund.
Hinweise

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

1 Introduction

In the United States, it has been estimated that as much as 11% of the population has been a victim of financial fraud (Anderson 2013) and that fraud costs the US economy as much as $50 billion per year (Deevy et al. 2012). While fraud is fundamentally a criminal problem, and therefore the result of individuals choosing to act in a criminal matter, in some cases potential victims may be able to mitigate the costs imposed on them by the criminal behavior. For example, although Bernie Madoff made fraudulent claims about his investments, it was clear to many observers at the time that the returns he claimed to be earning were so statistically unlikely that they could not be true. Indeed, in the ensuing scandal, many people were blamed as complicit because they did not appear to exhibit enough care in examining Madoff’s reported returns (Henriques 2012).
Skeptical investors were able to avoid involvement in the Madoff scandal by refusing to invest in his funds. After the fact, it appears that some potential investors chose not to invest with him because they believed Madoff was dishonest (Henriques 2012). If policy goals include not only preventing criminal financial fraud, but encouraging potential investors to protect themselves from it, it is natural to ask why some investors fall for scams like Madoff’s while others do not.
This research represents an entry into a relatively understudied area of financial literacy – how literacy affects skepticism and therefore fraud avoidance. We bring together literatures on financial literacy and deception in a new way to shed light on the tendency to fall victim to financial fraud.
In a simple investment task, students are asked to make an investment recommendation from among four hypothetical funds, including one based on Madoff’s fund. We use Truth Default Theory (TDT) (Levine 2014) to explain our results. Briefly, TDT argues that individuals generally assume other people are telling the truth, unless some discrepancy suggests otherwise. If a discrepancy is detected, then the individual may actively investigate the veracity of the other’s statements. We show that education increases the likelihood that students are suspicious of Madoff’s fund, and that for students whose suspicions are aroused, education makes them less likely to choose the Madoff fund.

2 Background

The question of how education affects fraud detection in financial decision-making is not only relevant for academic research, but also for policy implications and practical applications. In this section, we provide a background on the link between financial education, literacy and fraud, as well as our primary model for understanding fraud detection, Truth Default Theory. We also discuss the gaps in this field and how our research addresses those gaps.

2.1 Financial education and financial literacy

The literature on financial education and financial literacy suggests that they have positive effects on financial decision making. Lusardi and Mitchell (2014) reviewed studies that showed higher financial literacy was linked to better financial behavior. However, some researchers (Willis 2011; Hastings et al. 2013) challenged this relationship. Kaiser and Menkhoff (2017) and Kaiser and Menkhoff (2020) conducted meta-analyses of financial literacy studies and found evidence that financial education improved financial literacy and behavior. Despite some conflicting results, the general consensus is that more education and literacy enhance financial decision making. Therefore, it is reasonable to expect that greater financial understanding would also reduce the vulnerability to financial fraud.

2.2 Susceptibility to financial fraud

There are few academic papers on financial fraud, but some non-peer reviewed papers have been produced by organizations such as the Pension Research Council (e.g., Kieffer and Mottola 2016), which explored how individual characteristics influenced the likelihood of being defrauded. Knutson and Samanez-Larkin (2014) examined how personality traits affected skepticism and found that investors with lower impulse control were more prone to financial fraud, while cognitive ability and risk attitude did not matter. Anderson (2016) and Anderson (2013) found that consumer literacy and numeracy were associated with the ability to identify and avoid fraudulent advertising and schemes, respectively. Recently, some peer-reviewed papers have emerged on this topic. Andreou and Philip (2018), Engels et al. (2020), and Wei et al. (2021) all found that higher financial literacy helped individuals detect fraud in their financial transactions.

2.3 Truth Default Theory

Fraud is a form of deception, a topic that has been investigated more extensively in other social sciences. Levine (2014) has outlined what he calls Truth Default Theory (TDT), a comprehensive theory of deception and deception-detection. One key assertion of TDT is that people presume honesty in communication. When two people communicate, both people assume that the other person is telling the truth. While Bernie Madoff and the data presented on financial fraud provide obvious exceptions to this assumption, even a brief bit of introspection reveals its overall plausibility. Human beings communicate with each other constantly, and it is beyond our cognitive facilities to constantly monitor every bit of communication for deception. TDT argues that it is evolutionarily adaptive for humans to assume honesty, and Levine (2019) provides significant evidence in support of this argument.
If humans assume honesty, how is deception ever uncovered? TDT (illustrated in Fig. 1) argues some events push people into a state of suspicion, in which statements are evaluated for their honesty. Potential trigger events include the speaker having a motive for deception (like personal gain) or a lack of correspondence between communication content and some knowledge of reality. Once in the suspicious state, the person actively evaluates the veracity of the statements, taking account of the internal logic of the statements, and the correspondence between the statements and other known facts. If the statements fail to have internal logical consistency or contradict other information, the person updates his/her beliefs that the communication is honest. If this updating results in the level of perceived veracity falling below some threshold, then the statements are evaluated as dishonest.
TDT also helps to provide context for research by Zhang et al. (2015), who examined how cues could be used to help increase investors’ skepticism. In Zhang et al.’s decision task, survey respondents from Amazon MTurk were asked to play the role of a financial advisor and recommend a mutual fund for a hypothetical client. The survey participant had to choose between five different fictionalized funds, one of which was based on Madoff’s fund. Zhang et al. found that 68% of their respondents recommended the Madoff fund; however, when respondents were asked about which fund was most suspicious, this cue reduced the number choosing the Madoff fund to 51%. In terms of TDT, the cue acted as a trigger that helped to move some participants into a state of suspicion.
TDT helps explain the results presented earlier regarding the role of financial literacy. First, having more financial literacy could make people more likely to become suspicious in a situation involving fraud, and to investigate the fraudulent opportunity further. Second, having more financial literacy could also make people more skilled at detecting fraud once they are suspicious. In this paper, we present a decision task that is very similar to the one used by Zhang et al. (2015), but with students of economics and finance as participants.
The works by Andreou and Philip (2018), Engels et al. (2020) and Wei et al. (2021) represent all the published work in the fields of economics and finance on individual, non-professional investors and fraud detection. All those papers rely upon self-report surveys to investigate fraud detection. In particular, survey participants were asked if they had detected fraud in the past, and then this behavior was correlated with other collected variables like education, financial literacy, gender, etc. Importantly, all this data was self-reported. From the surveys we are able to learn if an individual thought that someone had tried to defraud them; however, we do not know if what the individual detected was actual fraud, nor do we know if other fraud attempts were successful. The current paper addresses this issue by presenting survey respondents with a decision task (based on Zhang et al. 2015) in which the nature of the fraud is known, allowing us to draw stronger conclusions about fraud detection. Furthermore, we advance the field of fraud detection by using TDT as our model – we are unaware of any papers in economics or finance that use this framework to understand fraud detection.

3 Methodology

3.1 Participants

The sample for this study consisted of undergraduate students from the College of Charleston.1 Students were recruited from a variety of finance and economics backgrounds, ranging from no prior coursework to extensive coursework. Students were offered extra credit in their courses in return for completing the survey. All students gave consent before starting the survey. Anonymity was guaranteed; credit was offered only for participation, not performance.
At the College of Charleston, the undergraduate economics and finance classes from which our sample was selected are taught in sequence, starting with Principles of Microeconomics, which introduces market dynamics and resource allocation. The next class is Principles of Macroeconomics, which focuses on aggregate economic analysis. The third class, Business Finance focuses on corporate finance fundamentals, leading to Intermediate Business Finance where students apply these principles using financial software. The sequence culminates in Applied Portfolio Management, where students actively manage diverse asset portfolios, integrating knowledge from all previous courses into real-world financial analysis and decision-making. Because enrollment in these sequenced classes is mutually exclusive, there was no chance that a student might be asked to take the survey twice.
A total of 479 students attempted the decision task, with 430 finishing the survey. Of those students, 390 undergraduates filled out all fields required for our analysis. The full dataset and survey instrument are available at https://​doi.​org/​10.​17632/​6d4y9g5jt4.​2 (Blackwell 2022).

3.2 Decision task

The basic structure of the survey was as follows:
1.
Main decision task
a.
Information on 4 investment options (plus the S&P 500 for reference)
 
b.
Selection of the participants’ preferred investment option
 
c.
Measures of suspiciousness and unethicality of investment options
 
 
2.
Big 5 Personality Inventory
 
3.
Socioeconomic information
 
4.
Educational background information
 
For the main decision task, survey participants were asked to make an investment recommendation from among four different hypothetical funds (whose returns are shown in Fig. 2 with S&P 500 for comparison).2 All the funds are based on real investments, although the fund names and dates are disguised (i.e. the data do not necessarily span between 2009 and 2014). One of the funds, “Fortitude Investments,” is based on Madoff’s fund. The main decision task was taken almost verbatim from the task presented by Zhang et al. (2015).
The participants were provided basic information about average returns and volatility for each fund. The subjects were then offered the opportunity to review more information about each fund. Choosing this option gave participants specific information about the fund’s investment strategy and auditors, with the Madoff fund’s auditor description listed as:
Fortitude uses SA & Associates, CPA for their auditing purposes. SA & Associates was established 15 years ago. The chief auditor was formerly a VP at Fortitude Investments.
This information provided the only additional opportunity to trigger the participants’ suspicion before submitting their recommendation. We collected data on the number of funds for which each participant asked for additional information as well as which funds they examined.
After submitting their recommendation, participants rated each fund based on how suspicious or unethical they found the fund to be, completed the Big Five Personality Inventory (John et al. 1991, 2008), and submitted information on their socio-economic and educational background. The post-decision data is discussed further below.

3.3 Data description

Descriptions of all variables used in our analysis are given in Table 1, with summary statistics for the analysis sample provided in Table 2. Our primary outcome measure is a binary variable indicating if the participant recommended the Madoff Fund or an alternative. We assume that participants who did not pick the Madoff fund did so because they detected potential fraud from that mutual fund. Participants provided demographic data by self-reporting their age, gender, and race. Because there may be a difference in the effect of additional information depending on whether the Madoff Fund is one of the funds being compared, we split this variable into those who included the Madoff Fund in their examination and those who excluded it.
Table 1
Variable definitions
Variable
Definition
Madoff
Indicator variable which equals 1 if participant chose Madoff Fund
SUS
Participant response to question regarding how suspicious Madoff Fund is, ranging from relative suspiciousness of the Madoff Fund; equal to a Likert value from 1 (least suspicious) to 7 (most suspicious) assigned to the Madoff Fund, divided by the mean score of the same type assigned to all funds
Duration
Time spent (in seconds) by participant completing the survey
Age
Age in years
Race & Gender
Indicator variables which equal 1 if the participant self-identified into the listed category
Female
Female
Asian
Asian
Caucasian
Caucasian
Hispanic
Hispanic
Multi-
Multi-racial
Other
Native-American or Other
BFI
Score on Big 5 Inventory for the listed trait (Source: American Psychological Association 2019)
Extra
Extraversion; APA Definition: “characterized by an orientation of one’s interests and energies toward the outer world of people and things rather than the inner world of subjective experience”
Agree
Agreeableness; APA Definition: “the tendency to act in a cooperative, unselfish manner”
Con
Conscientiousness; APA Definition: “the tendency to be organized, responsible, and hardworking”
Neuro
Neuroticism; APA Definition: “characterized by a chronic level of emotional instability and proneness to psychological distress”
Open
Openness; APA Definition: “the tendency to be open to new aesthetic, cultural, or intellectual experiences”
Education
Indicator variables which equal 1 if the participant has completed the listed course in the sequence or higher (i.e. a participant with a 1 for “Macro” will also have a 1 for “Micro”)
Micro
Principles of Microeconomics; 1st in sequence
Macro
Principles of Macroeconomics; 2nd in sequence, with Micro as prerequisite
Finance
Business Finance; 3rd in sequence, with Macro as prerequisite
Intermed
Intermediate Business Finance; 4th in sequence, with Finance as prerequisite
Portfolio
Applied Portfolio Management; 5th in sequence, with Intermed as prerequisite
Information
Count variables of the number of funds for which the participant sought more info
Excluding
Counts if participant did not request info on the Madoff fund; ranges from 0 to 3
Including
Counts if participant requested info on the Madoff fund; ranges from 0 to 3
on race: Participants self-identified into one of seven categories—African American, Asian, Caucasian, Hispanic, Multi-Racial, Native American, and Other. Only one participant in the complete sample identified as Native American, so this category was combined with Other, leaving six categories to include as dummies. African American was taken as the excluded dummy, such that dummies indicate differences between the listed group and African American participants
on gender: Participants self-identified into one of three gender categories—Male, Female, and Other. No participants identified as Other
Table 2
Descriptive statistics (n = 370)
Variable
Mean
Std Dev
Min
Max
Madoff
0.47
0.50
0
1
SUS
1.00
0.44
0.25
2.8
Duration
1996.73
11376.82
186
170604
Demographics
  Age
20.61
2.28
10
42
  Female
0.55
0.50
0
1
  Asian
0.02
0.13
0
1
  Caucasian
0.84
0.37
0
1
  Hispanic
0.03
0.18
0
1
  Multi-
0.03
0.17
0
1
  Other
0.04
0.18
0
1
BFI
  Extra
3.52
0.74
1.38
5.00
  Agree
3.83
0.59
2.00
5.00
  Con
3.65
0.58
1.89
5.00
  Neuro
2.80
0.68
1.00
4.75
  Open
3.57
0.52
1.90
4.80
Education
  Micro
0.91
0.29
0
1
  Macro
0.66
0.47
0
1
  Finance
0.36
0.48
0
1
  Intermed
0.14
0.34
0
1
  Portfolio
0.09
0.28
0
1
Information
  Excluding
0.29
0.69
0
3
  Including
1.92
1.62
0
4
For educational information, participants indicated which sequenced economics and finance classes they have completed. We used this data to create cumulative course indicators. This means the total effect of education for a participant who has completed Business Finance will also include the effect of Principles of Micro- and Macroeconomics, and coefficient estimates indicate the marginal effects of completing each course.
The Big Five Personality Inventory (BFI) measures participants on the five traits of extraversion, conscientiousness, agreeableness, neuroticism, and openness. The BFI consists of statements with which each individual could agree or disagree. An example BFI statement is: “I am someone who is talkative” (John et al. 1991). A person who agrees with this statement would rank highly for extraversion. The American Psychological Association Dictionary definitions for the BFI traits are included in Table 1.
To measure skepticism, we followed Zhang et al. (2015) by asking students to rank how suspicious they found each fund. To avoid triggering scrutiny due to asking the question, the question was left until after participants had submitted their recommendation. To ensure that these scores are comparable across observations, we created the variable SUS by dividing the score assigned to the Madoff Fund by the average of all scores provided by each participant.
Finally, our dataset includes the time taken to complete the survey in seconds. While almost all the 390 undergraduates who filled out all required fields took between 3 and 30 min to complete it, 20 participants completed the survey in less than 3 min. These participants chose all five fund options in nearly equal proportions, suggesting that all their answers may have been randomly selected to complete the survey as quickly as possible. To avoid introducing random noise into all variables used, we drop these 20 observations from our primary analysis sample.
In the remaining 370 observations of our analysis sample, the median duration was 434 s, or slightly over 7 min. The mean duration was 1,997 s (more than 33 min), reflecting a small number of extreme outliers. We include a dummy variable for participants who took more than 30 min to control for these outliers.3

4 Hypotheses

4.1 Truth Default Theory

According to TDT, deception and fraud are extraordinarily difficult to detect under most circumstances. Only those who are triggered to scrutinize have a real chance to detect fraud, and even then, only if enough inconsistencies can be identified. In the context of our investment task, this suggests a two-stage relationship between education, skepticism, and detecting the fraud of the Madoff Fund. In the first stage, the level of skepticism is determined. In the second stage, participants choose, conditional on their level of skepticism, to recommend either the Madoff Fund or an alternative.
If the participant has not been triggered to scrutinize for possible deception, they will take the information presented at face value in the second stage. In this “trusting” case, we would expect additional information about the fund options to reinforce the benefits of the high-return, low-risk Madoff Fund, increasing the likelihood of recommending it. Similarly, more economics and finance coursework should enable the participant to recognize the desirability of these fund traits, also increasing the likelihood of recommending the Madoff Fund.
If the participant has been triggered to scrutinize, however, we would expect the opposite effects. Since deception detection relies on recognizing inconsistencies between the deception and other known information, additional information about the fund options and additional background in economics and finance should provide more opportunities to detect the deception, which should reduce the likelihood of recommending the Madoff Fund.
Mathematically, TDT suggests a model for fund choice such as the following:
$${\varvec{P}}\left({\varvec{M}}{\varvec{a}}{\varvec{d}}{\varvec{o}}{\varvec{f}}{\varvec{f}}|{\varvec{S}}{\varvec{U}}{\varvec{S}}\right)=\left\{\begin{array}{c}f\left({\varvec{E}}{\varvec{d}}{\varvec{u}}{\varvec{c}}{\varvec{a}}{\varvec{t}}{\varvec{i}}{\varvec{o}}{\varvec{n}},\boldsymbol{ }{\varvec{I}}{\varvec{n}}{\varvec{f}}{\varvec{o}}{\varvec{r}}{\varvec{m}}{\varvec{a}}{\varvec{t}}{\varvec{i}}{\varvec{o}}{\varvec{n}},{\varvec{C}}{\varvec{o}}{\varvec{n}}{\varvec{t}}{\varvec{r}}{\varvec{o}}{\varvec{l}}{\varvec{s}}\right) if\, SUS\le Threshold\\ g\left({\varvec{E}}{\varvec{d}}{\varvec{u}}{\varvec{c}}{\varvec{a}}{\varvec{t}}{\varvec{i}}{\varvec{o}}{\varvec{n}},\boldsymbol{ }{\varvec{I}}{\varvec{n}}{\varvec{f}}{\varvec{o}}{\varvec{r}}{\varvec{m}}{\varvec{a}}{\varvec{t}}{\varvec{i}}{\varvec{o}}{\varvec{n}},{\varvec{C}}{\varvec{o}}{\varvec{n}}{\varvec{t}}{\varvec{r}}{\varvec{o}}{\varvec{l}}{\varvec{s}}\right) if\, SUS>Threshold\end{array}\right.$$
(1)
where f(·) is increasing in both education and information, g(·) is decreasing in both, and controls include demographic and personality factors which may also affect the choice of fund.

4.2 Determinants of skepticism

Since the functional form of the binary choice of fund is dependent on the level of skepticism, the first stage involves identifying what determines suspicion. TDT does not make sharp predictions about what determines initial suspicion, but as it seems likely that individuals may differ in levels of innate skepticism, we control for personality and demographics. While we would not expect education to have a significant effect on innate skepticism directly, familiarity with economics and finance should make participants more confident in requesting additional financial information. We hypothesize that the effect of education on SUS should be mediated by information gathering.

5 Results

Table 3 shows the proportion of students choosing each of the five funds. Forty-seven percent of participants in our analysis sample chose the Madoff fund. Interestingly, more than 5% of students chose the S&P 500, even though the S&P data was only provided for comparison purposes.
Table 3
Fund choice
Fund
% choosing fund
(all complete surveys)
% choosing fund
(analysis sample)
Tobacco Trade
5.81
5.41
Power Trade
24.65
25.68
Madoff
45.58
47.03
Alpha
17.21
16.76
S&P 500
6.74
5.14
Figure 3 shows how the Madoff decision is related to educational background. Participants who had not completed any coursework in economics or finance were the most likely to choose Madoff, and participants who had completed Business Finance were less likely to choose Madoff than those with only one or two courses. There appears to be a u-shape in education, however, as participants who have completed Intermediate Business Finance or Applied Portfolio Management were more likely to choose Madoff than those who have only completed Business Finance. This u-shape may be the result of the relatively small number of students in the analysis sample who have taken the higher-level courses, making the relationship with education difficult to generalize from this simple comparison. If we divide the subjects into those who have taken finance and those who have not, 43% of the students with finance chose Madoff, while 49% of the students without finance chose Madoff.
While these preliminary results suggest that choosing the Madoff Fund depends on education, personality, and demographics, a more formal analysis is required to see the role of skepticism and test our TDT-related hypotheses.

5.1 Threshold model of TDT

TDT suggests a structural break in the probability of recommending the Madoff Fund, with the coefficients related to education and information changing between those who are trusting and those who become skeptical. However, there is no obvious a priori value of SUS that should be used to test such a break. Since TDT claims that most individuals have a high threshold, a high level of suspicion may be required to ensure sufficient ‘state’ skepticism. On the other hand, the context of the survey and the type of students who select into economics and finance courses may suggest a high unobservable innate skepticism, requiring only a modest level of suspicion to trigger scrutiny.
The simplest econometric approach to searching for a breakpoint is a threshold linear probability model. This extends the method of ordinary least squares (OLS) regression, which chooses coefficients that minimize the sum of squared residuals (SSR), to the choice of breakpoint. The data are sorted by relative suspicion, split into high and low suspicion groups at all possible values of the threshold variable, and linear probability regressions are run for each possible split. The model that provides the lowest SSR is then reported.4
The linear probability model results are provided in Table 4. The threshold for SUS is 0.57, placing less than 15% of the analysis sample in the trusting group. Extroverted and female participants are still less likely to choose Madoff, while conscientious participants are more likely to do so.
Table 4
Truth default theory: binary choice for Madoff fund
 
Linear probability model
Logit model
Variable
Expanded
Analysis sample
Pooled
With break
I(Dur > 1800)
0.247***
(0.086)
0.246***
(0.085)
1.288***
(0.497)
1.129**
(0.514)
I(Dur < 1800)
-0.141
(0.105)
Demographics
  Age
-0.012
(0.012)
-0.010
(0.012)
-0.001
(0.051)
-0.041
(0.061)
  Female
-0.129**
(0.052)
-0.130**
(0.054)
-0.484*
(0.265)
-0.624**
(0.289)
  Asian
-0.270*
(0.141)
-0.302**
(0.140)
-2.184**
(0.952)
-2.057**
(0.940)
  Caucasian
-0.015
(0.108)
-0.037
(0.110)
-0.061
(0.496)
-0.168
(0.524)
  Hispanic
-0.045
(0.179)
-0.063
(0.181)
-0.204
(0.912)
-0.380
(0.933)
  Multi-
0.149
(0.173)
0.128
(0.173)
0.440
(0.823)
0.380
(0.851)
  Other
0.129
(0.163)
0.099
(0.177)
0.350
(0.800)
0.440
(0.929)
BFI
  Extra
-0.070**
(0.032)
-0.070**
(0.033)
-0.349**
(0.164)
-0.367**
(0.179)
  Agree
-0.033
(0.046)
-0.040
(0.046)
-0.184
(0.218)
-0.166
(0.244)
  Con
0.103**
(0.044)
0.105**
(0.044)
0.428*
(0.221)
0.555**
(0.255)
  Neuro
0.009
(0.039)
0.005
(0.039)
-0.096
(0.195)
0.008
(0.210)
  Open
0.031
(0.045)
0.029
(0.046)
0.163
(0.231)
0.126
(0.251)
Trusting
(SUS ≤ 0.57, n = 53)
(SUS ≤ 0.57, n = 51)
    
  Constant
0.835*
(0.441)
0.874**
(0.445)
  
-1.993
(2.533)
Education
  Micro
-0.022
(0.176)
0.023
(0.178)
  
1.718
(1.299)
  Macro
0.021
(0.123)
0.021
(0.124)
  
0.670
(1.171)
  Finance
0.192**
(0.098)
0.159
(0.100)
  
14.573**
(1.280)
  Intermed
-0.347
(0.241)
-0.318
(0.237)
  
-19.526**
(2.762)
  Portfolio
-0.067
(0.276)
-0.091
(0.273)
  
1.738
(1.813)
Information
  Excluding
0.224***
(0.080)
0.185**
(0.082)
  
14.963***
(1.292)
  Including
0.067**
(0.034)
0.046
(0.037)
  
1.410**
(0.651)
Skeptical
(SUS > 0.57, n = 337)
(SUS > 0.57, n = 319)
    
  Constant
0.820**
(0.393)
0.837**
(0.394)
1.049
(1.919)
3.697**
(1.577)
Education
  Micro
-0.120
(0.102)
-0.135
(0.102)
-0.454
(0.450)
-2.382*
(1.390)
  Macro
0.017
(0.071)
0.019
(0.072)
0.189
(0.303)
-0.695
(1.221)
  Finance
-0.117*
(0.066)
-0.111
(0.070)
-0.545*
(0.315)
-15.098***
(1.317)
  Intermed
0.149
(0.154)
0.131
(0.154)
0.301
(0.615)
20.294***
(2.842)
  Portfolio
-0.056
(0.166)
-0.057
(0.168)
-0.209
(0.668)
-2.073
(1.960)
Information
  Excluding
-0.186***
(0.033)
-0.203***
(0.033)
-0.856***
(0.242)
-16.253***
(1.315)
  Including
-0.037**
(0.018)
-0.033*
(0.019)
-0.083
(0.083)
-1.597**
(0.659)
Observations
390
370
370
370
R2 & Pseudo R2
0.255
0.257
0.089
0.222
Dependent variable is Madoff, described in Table 1 Variable Definitions. I(Dur > 1800) is a dummy variable for participants who took more than 30 min to complete the survey. All other variable descriptions are in Table 1 Variable Definitions. Heteroscedasticity-robust standard errors are in parentheses. * denotes statistical significance at the 10% level, ** is 5% level, *** is 1% level
Gathering more information seems consistent with TDT. For the trusting group, asking for more fund information without comparing it to the Madoff Fund increases the likelihood of choosing Madoff. For the skeptical group, the coefficient is similar in magnitude but opposite in sign, indicating that gathering more information makes choosing Madoff less likely for those who are scrutinizing.5
The effect of education is less obvious, with no statistically significant education coefficients in our sample. This may be due to the small sample, as expanding the sample to include all undergraduates with the required data suggests that completing the Business Finance course has a positive and significant (5% level) effect, increasing the likelihood of choosing Madoff. For the skeptical group in the expanded sample, having completed the same course reduces the likelihood of choosing Madoff, although this effect is only significant at the 10% level.
To properly determine if there is a difference in the effect of education between the trusting and skeptical groups, we must test for differences in corresponding coefficients in our analysis sample. The χ2-statistic for difference in coefficients is 5.00, which is statistically significant at the 5% level. Although we cannot confidently say completing Business Finance makes choosing Madoff more likely for the trusting group and less likely for the skeptical, there is a difference between the groups in the direction suggested by TDT.
None of the other education variables show a significant difference in coefficients. This suggests that the skills that differentiate the choices of trusting and skeptical students are not acquired after a single introduction to economic and finance coursework. This seems reasonable since only basic financial concepts are covered in the two economics courses studied. Key models such as the CAPM are not covered in detail until Business Finance.

5.2 Logit model of TDT

One possible shortcoming of this analysis is the use of linear probability models, which have a variety of known shortcomings. One popular alternative for binary choices is the logit model, which we also present in Table 4. We consider versions with no break (pooled) and with the same break in coefficients suggested by the threshold model.
The signs and general significance of gender, extraversion, and conscientiousness match the threshold model, both in the pooled model and the model with the break. In the pooled model, both completing Business Finance and gathering more information reduce the likelihood of choosing Madoff, although the effect of the course is only significant at the 10% level.
The break is highly statistically significant (χ2 = 1,370), providing strong evidence that skepticism alters some or all the effects of education and information gathering. We again see statistically significant differences (and signs that match TDT) in the effect of the Business Finance course and gathering information between the trusting group and the skeptical group.
The magnitudes of the coefficients on Business Finance are very close, and we cannot reject the null that they sum to zero (χ2 = 2.2). If financial education increases the likelihood of already-skeptical students avoiding a Ponzi scheme, it is difficult to identify such an effect in our data. However, gathering information, whether excluding or including information about the Madoff Fund, seems to not just cancel out but actively reduce the likelihood of choosing Madoff (χ2 = 19.4 and 4.3, respectively).
Finally, the effect of the Intermediate Business Finance course in the break model deserves some comment. In both groups, the coefficient seems to have a dampening or corrective response to that of the previous course. In the case of the trusting group, we can reject the null that the coefficients sum to zero (χ2 = 5.6) at the 5% level, suggesting that students with more practice using workhorse finance models are less inclined to jump at a fund that promises high returns for free. However, we are unable to reject the null that the effect among skeptical students sums to zero (χ2 = 0.1).

5.3 Skepticism and due diligence

Given the evidence that skepticism has an important effect on fund recommendation in our decision task, we now consider our first-stage hypotheses. Results of an OLS regression of SUS on our demographic, personality, education, and information variables are presented in Table 5.
Table 5
Skepticism estimation: participant disposition toward Madoff fund
 
SUS
Information
Variable
 
Poisson
Ordered logit
Constant
0.984**
(0.441)
0.358
(0.528)
I(Dur > 1800)
-0.086
(0.078)
-0.019
(0.115)
-0.107
(0.316)
Demographics
  Age
-0.023
(0.017)
-0.003
(0.015)
0.003
(0.056)
  Female
-0.081
(0.054)
-0.153**
(0.074)
-0.484**
(0.223)
  Asian
0.161
(0.142)
-0.112
(0.358)
-0.339
(1.142)
  Caucasian
-0.012
(0.097)
-0.034
(0.176)
-0.095
(0.530)
  Hispanic
0.011
(0.131)
0.056
(0.202)
-0.028
(0.601)
  Multi-
0.275
(0.204)
-0.353
(0.281)
-0.777
(0.683)
  Other
0.092
(0.131)
0.190
(0.225)
0.575
(0.738)
BFI
  Extra
0.064*
(0.038)
-0.070
(0.044)
-0.230*
(0.132)
  Agree
0.000
(0.048)
0.090
(0.062)
0.290
(0.187)
  Con
0.051
(0.040)
0.094
(0.063)
0.254
(0.195)
  Neuro
0.039
(0.037)
0.049
(0.057)
0.163
(0.178)
  Open
-0.009
(0.044)
-0.036
(0.062)
-0.091
(0.200)
Education
  Micro
-0.145*
(0.075)
0.105
(0.143)
0.271
(0.390)
  Macro
0.051
(0.059)
-0.030
(0.095)
-0.058
(0.259)
  Finance
0.048
(0.067)
0.121
(0.094)
0.319
(0.266)
  Intermed
0.137
(0.159)
0.336***
(0.100)
1.389***
(0.430)
  Portfolio
-0.094
(0.162)
-0.266**
(0.113)
-1.068**
(0.506)
Information
  Excluding
0.015
(0.030)
    
  Including
0.055***
(0.017)
    
Observations
370
370
370
R2 & Pseudo R2
0.090
0.021
0.03
SUS indicates an OLS regression the relative suspiciousness measure defined in Table 1 Variable Definitions as dependent variable. Information indicates the dependent variable is the number of funds for which participants request more information. I(Dur > 1800) is a dummy variable for participants who took more than 30 min to complete the survey. All other variable descriptions are in Table 1. Heteroscedasticity-robust standard errors are in parentheses. * denotes statistical significance at the 10% level, ** is 5% level, *** is 1% level
The constant term for the ordered logit regression has been suppressed
We see that none of the demographic variables are statistically significant, and among the BFI only extraversion is significant (at the 10% level). Among the education variables, only the Principles of Microeconomics course is significant, also at the 10% level.
The negative sign on this coefficient may be concerning since we would not like to think an economics course makes students less adept at detecting fraud. However, this may reflect any number of driving forces. Since students in this course have only just been introduced to basic ideas like volatility, such students may simply associate high risk with suspiciousness.
In contrast to the other groups of explanatory variables, asking for additional information has a strong and clear effect on the perceived relative suspiciousness of the Madoff Fund. When the Madoff Fund is included among the funds for which more information is considered, each additional piece of information increases how suspicious the Madoff Fund seems. This act of comparing the Madoff Fund’s information to multiple alternatives could be viewed as financial due diligence and may represent the participants actively scrutinizing returns that seem too good to be true.
These results broadly align with the interpretation of the SUS variable as capturing ‘state’ rather than ‘trait’ skepticism. To examine our hypothesis that the effect of education on ‘state’ skepticism should be mediated through information gathering, we consider a regression of requests for additional information on education and our controls. Since our dependent variable is count data, we use Poisson and ordered logit regressions.
Of our controls, only gender is statistically significant in both models. Female participants are less likely to ask for more information than male participants, which is curious given that female participants are also less likely to choose Madoff.
The impact of education on information gathering is present but is more complex than expected. We do find that students with a finance background are more likely to request additional information, but the effect does not become statistically significant until students have completed Intermediate Business Finance. Even more unexpectedly, this effect seems to be reduced or canceled entirely for students who have also taken the Applied Portfolio Management course (χ2 = 0.34 for the Poisson regression and 0.57 for the ordered logit).
Likely causes for this bounce back are not immediately obvious. Perhaps these students have such a high degree of confidence that they do not think the additional information will be necessary. This finding is similar to what the professional skepticism literature shows: less experienced auditors are more skeptical than more experienced ones. However, this does not mean that more experience or knowledge reduces skepticism. It could mean that more advanced students have learned to select the most relevant information for their investment decisions and ignore the less important ones. Or perhaps the bounce back is simply the result of normal variation among a small number of participants who fall into these two highest education categories (18 for Intermediate and 32 for Portfolio). Whatever the explanation, the results seem to align with TDT-based predictions, but only up to a point.

6 Discussion

One of the most common and important claims made by higher education is that it teaches critical thinking skills. Skepticism regarding investment seems to be one area where this claim should apply, especially in the context of economics and finance education. In this paper we have tried to evaluate this claim. We find limited evidence that education impacts participants’ investment decisions. The results of the threshold model suggest that students use information from their finance classes.
It is perhaps surprising that education did not have a larger impact. However, none of the classes we examined explicitly teach fraud detection. Basic finance presents CAPM, which implies the risk-return profile of the Madoff fund is suspicious. But in no finance classes are students taught to look for fabricated returns. Without more explicit training, students may default to trusting what they see. However, the small impacts of education on skepticism need to be set in proper context. According to TDT, it is difficult to detect any fraud because humans are strongly biased to believe each other. That we observe some small effects in this context is encouraging regarding financial fraud detection by educated investors.
Compared to the results generated by Zhang et al. (2015), our participants are less likely to choose the Madoff fund overall. Zhang et al. reported that 68% of their respondents chose the Madoff fund, compared to our rate of 46%. Their participants came from Amazon MTurk, and although they did not report the average education level of their respondents, it was most likely lower than our sample. This suggests education may have a greater impact on skepticism in financial decision-making that our data does not capture.
In this paper we wanted to limit our analysis to investors who are not specifically trained to detect fraud. However, there is a substantial literature in accounting on fraud detection. Our paper is consistent with a basic finding in that literature – more education and expertise lead to a higher likelihood of identifying fraud (e.g. Grenier 2011; Plumlee et al. 2012; Carpenter et al. 2011).
One of the limitations of this study is the lack of information on participants’ risk attitudes. Some recent research indicates risk attitudes are correlated with some of the BFI personality traits (Frey et al. 2017; Andersson et al. 2020), while other research suggests that education and risk attitudes both affect each other (see Outreville 2015, for a review). Given these relationships, our regression estimates of the impact of education and personality may be subject to omitted variable bias. Future research should investigate how risk attitudes impact the Madoff decision.
In terms of policy, we interpret these results as supportive of the idea that more education in economics and finance should reduce the likelihood that consumers are victimized by financial fraud. However, given the human propensity to believe most of what we are told, education can only play a limited role and must be part of a broader set of strategies to deal with fraud.

Declarations

Conflict of interest

None of the authors have any conflicts of interest to declare.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://​creativecommons.​org/​licenses/​by/​4.​0/​.

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Fußnoten
1
Human subjects approval was granted by the College of Charleston’s Institutional Review Board, for Protocol IRB-2018–03-14–083129.
 
2
Note that our decision task required participants to make a recommendation for a client, not make an investment choice for themselves. A recent meta-analysis by Polman and Wu (2020) indicates that generally, people are willing to take on more risk when making decisions for others. However, when making decisions for clients, people are slightly more conservative than when making decisions for themselves.
 
3
We also ran our analysis dropping the 26 observations (about 7% of our sample) with a duration greater than 30 min. This loss of data reduced the statistical significance of some results without substantially altering the coefficient estimates for our variables of interest.
 
4
The Stata 17 command used, threshold, selects based on the Bayesian Information Criterion, which is a penalized version of SSR.
 
5
We also considered modeling the threshold effect of education without the inclusion of information gathering variables. The impact of this change is negligible: none of the education variables that lack statistical significance gain it by dropping information, and no statistically significant variables change sign. Considering the risk of introducing omitted variable bias by dropping the information variables, we retain these variables in our reported TDT models and our model of suspicion below.
 
Literatur
Zurück zum Zitat Anderson K (2013) Consumer fraud in the United States, 2011: the third FTC survey. The Federal Trade Commission, Washington, DC Anderson K (2013) Consumer fraud in the United States, 2011: the third FTC survey. The Federal Trade Commission, Washington, DC
Zurück zum Zitat Andersson O, Holm HJ, Tyran J-R, Wengström E (2020) Robust inference in risk elicitation tasks. J Risk Uncertain 61:195–209CrossRef Andersson O, Holm HJ, Tyran J-R, Wengström E (2020) Robust inference in risk elicitation tasks. J Risk Uncertain 61:195–209CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Andreou PC, Philip D (2018) Cyprus Economic Policy Review 12(2): 3–23 Andreou PC, Philip D (2018) Cyprus Economic Policy Review 12(2): 3–23
Zurück zum Zitat Carpenter TD, Durtschi C, Gaynor LM (2011) The incremental benefits of a forensic accounting course on skepticism and fraud-related judgments. Issues Account Educ 26(1):1–21CrossRef Carpenter TD, Durtschi C, Gaynor LM (2011) The incremental benefits of a forensic accounting course on skepticism and fraud-related judgments. Issues Account Educ 26(1):1–21CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Deevy M, Lucich S, Beals M (2012) Scams, schemes and swindles: a review of consumer financial fraud research. Financial Fraud Research Center, Palo Alto Deevy M, Lucich S, Beals M (2012) Scams, schemes and swindles: a review of consumer financial fraud research. Financial Fraud Research Center, Palo Alto
Zurück zum Zitat Engels C, Kumar K, Philip D (2020) Financial literacy and fraud detection. Eur J Financ 26(4–5):420–442CrossRef Engels C, Kumar K, Philip D (2020) Financial literacy and fraud detection. Eur J Financ 26(4–5):420–442CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Frey R, Pedroni A, Mata R, Rieskamp J, Hertwig R (2017) Risk preference shares the psychometric structure ofmajor psychological traits. Sci Adv 3:e1701381 Frey R, Pedroni A, Mata R, Rieskamp J, Hertwig R (2017) Risk preference shares the psychometric structure ofmajor psychological traits. Sci Adv 3:e1701381
Zurück zum Zitat Grenier JH (2011) Encouraging professional skepticism in the industry specialization era. Working paper, Miami University Grenier JH (2011) Encouraging professional skepticism in the industry specialization era. Working paper, Miami University
Zurück zum Zitat Hastings JS, Madrian BC, Skimmyhorn WL (2013) Financial literacy, financial education, and economic outcomes. Ann Rev Econ 5:347–373CrossRef Hastings JS, Madrian BC, Skimmyhorn WL (2013) Financial literacy, financial education, and economic outcomes. Ann Rev Econ 5:347–373CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Henriques D (2012) The wizard of lies. St. Martin’s Press, New York Henriques D (2012) The wizard of lies. St. Martin’s Press, New York
Zurück zum Zitat John OP, Donahue EM, Kentle RL (1991) The Big five inventory-versions 4a and 54. University of California, Berkeley, Institute of Personality and Social Research, Berkeley, CA John OP, Donahue EM, Kentle RL (1991) The Big five inventory-versions 4a and 54. University of California, Berkeley, Institute of Personality and Social Research, Berkeley, CA
Zurück zum Zitat John OP, Naumann LP, Soto CJ (2008) Paradigm shift to the integrative Big Five trait taxonomy: history, measurement, and conceptual issues. In: John OP, Robins RW, Pervin LA (eds) Handbook of personality: theory and research. Guilford Press, New York, NY, pp 114–158 John OP, Naumann LP, Soto CJ (2008) Paradigm shift to the integrative Big Five trait taxonomy: history, measurement, and conceptual issues. In: John OP, Robins RW, Pervin LA (eds) Handbook of personality: theory and research. Guilford Press, New York, NY, pp 114–158
Zurück zum Zitat Kaiser T, Menkhoff L (2020) Financial education in schools: a meta-analysis of experimental studies. Econ Educ Rev 78:101930 Kaiser T, Menkhoff L (2020) Financial education in schools: a meta-analysis of experimental studies. Econ Educ Rev 78:101930
Zurück zum Zitat Kaiser T, Menkhoff L (2017) Does financial education impact financial literacy and financial behavior, and if so, when? World Bank Econ Rev 31(3):611–630CrossRef Kaiser T, Menkhoff L (2017) Does financial education impact financial literacy and financial behavior, and if so, when? World Bank Econ Rev 31(3):611–630CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Kieffer CN, Mottola GR (2016) Understanding and combating investment fraud. Pension Research Council Working Paper Kieffer CN, Mottola GR (2016) Understanding and combating investment fraud. Pension Research Council Working Paper
Zurück zum Zitat Knutson B, Samanez-Larkin G (2014) Individual differences in susceptibility to investment fraud. Stanford University, Palo Alto. Working Paper Knutson B, Samanez-Larkin G (2014) Individual differences in susceptibility to investment fraud. Stanford University, Palo Alto. Working Paper
Zurück zum Zitat Levine TR (2014) Truth-Default Theory (TDT): a theory of human deception and deception detection. J Lang Soc Psychol 33(4):378–392CrossRef Levine TR (2014) Truth-Default Theory (TDT): a theory of human deception and deception detection. J Lang Soc Psychol 33(4):378–392CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Levine TR (2019) Duped: Truth-Default Theory and the social science of lying and deception. University of Alabama Press, Tuscaloosa, AL Levine TR (2019) Duped: Truth-Default Theory and the social science of lying and deception. University of Alabama Press, Tuscaloosa, AL
Zurück zum Zitat Lusardi A, Mitchell OS (2014) The economic importance of financial literacy: theory and evidence. J Econ Lit 52(1):5–44CrossRef Lusardi A, Mitchell OS (2014) The economic importance of financial literacy: theory and evidence. J Econ Lit 52(1):5–44CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Outreville JF (2015) The relationship between relative risk aversion and the level of education: a survey and implications for the demand for life insurance. J Econ Surv 29(1):97–111CrossRef Outreville JF (2015) The relationship between relative risk aversion and the level of education: a survey and implications for the demand for life insurance. J Econ Surv 29(1):97–111CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Plumlee D, Rixom BA, Rosman AJ (2012) Training auditors to think skeptically. Working paper, The University of Utah, and University of Connecticut Plumlee D, Rixom BA, Rosman AJ (2012) Training auditors to think skeptically. Working paper, The University of Utah, and University of Connecticut
Zurück zum Zitat Polman E, Wu K (2020) Decision making for others involving risk: a review and meta-analysis. J Econ Psychol 77:102184CrossRef Polman E, Wu K (2020) Decision making for others involving risk: a review and meta-analysis. J Econ Psychol 77:102184CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Wei Li, Peng M, Weixing Wu (2021) Financial literacy and fraud detection – Evidence from China. Int Rev Econ Financ 76:478–494CrossRef Wei Li, Peng M, Weixing Wu (2021) Financial literacy and fraud detection – Evidence from China. Int Rev Econ Financ 76:478–494CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Willis LE (2011) The financial education fallacy. American Economic Review 101(3):429–434CrossRef Willis LE (2011) The financial education fallacy. American Economic Review 101(3):429–434CrossRef
Zurück zum Zitat Zhang T, Fletcher PO, GinoF, Bazerman MH (2015) Reducing bounded ethicality: how to help individuals notice and avoid unethical behavior. Special issue on bad behavior. Organ Dynam 44:4, 310–317 Zhang T, Fletcher PO, GinoF, Bazerman MH (2015) Reducing bounded ethicality: how to help individuals notice and avoid unethical behavior. Special issue on bad behavior. Organ Dynam 44:4, 310–317
Metadaten
Titel
Who gets duped? The impact of education on fraud detection in an investment task
verfasst von
Calvin Blackwell
Norman Maynard
James Malm
Mark Pyles
Marcia Snyder
Mark Witte
Publikationsdatum
10.05.2024
Verlag
Springer US
Erschienen in
Journal of Economics and Finance
Print ISSN: 1055-0925
Elektronische ISSN: 1938-9744
DOI
https://doi.org/10.1007/s12197-024-09672-z